588 Glenhuntly Rd, Elsternwick 3185

TELEHEALTH AND TELEPHONE SESSIONS AVAILABLE

Blog Layout

COVID-19 Messaging - Don't Personalise ... Collectivise!

Franco Greco • Mar 14, 2020

Is the worth appealing to individual or collective interests? What is really needed? Look at Sth Korea compared to Italy ... one a collectivist culture ... the other an individualistic culture ... different responses ... different outcomes.

In Italy, millions are locked down and more than 1,000 people have died from the coronavirus. In South Korea, hit by the disease at about the same time, only a few thousand are quarantined and 67 people have died. As the virus courses through the world, the story of two outbreaks illustrates a coming problem for countries now grappling with an explosion in cases.

Both countries saw their first cases of the disease called Covid-19 in late January.  South Korea has since reported 67 deaths out of nearly 8,000 confirmed cases, after testing more than 222,000 people. In contrast, Italy has had 1,016 deaths and identified more than 15,000 cases after carrying out more than 73,000 tests on an unspecified number of people.

Epidemiologists say it is impossible to compare the numbers directly. But some say the dramatically different outcomes point to an important insight: aggressive and sustained testing is a powerful tool for fighting the virus.

Individualise ... or Collectivise?

According to Professor Stephen Reicher, University of St. Andrews:

"The way we deal with the coronavirus is bound up with the way we think about society and about the individual."

The commonsense assumption, which is reflected in the advice being handed out to the public, is that the way to change behaviour is to appeal to individual interests.

To make sure people take notice, personalise the message: ‘change your behaviour so that you will survive’. 

Surely that makes sense? Well no. It is precisely the wrong thing to do. Here’s why.

At a practical level, those least at risk (young, fit, healthy) may well feel it isn’t worthwhile to make the necessary changes and so continue to act in ways that put the most vulnerable (old and infirm) at risk of infection. 

How many people have you heard say:

"It will be just a cold ... so what ... I will get over it ..."

Additionally, at a moral level, we have the right to disregard dangers to ourselves and some even glory in being risk takers.

It might be foolish, but it isn’t disreputable to ignore safety advice.

On top of this, if we frame things individually – look after yourself! – we run into difficulties when it comes to getting people to behave in ways that are inconvenient to themselves but benefit others (self quarantining, for instance).

The same goes when it comes to distributing scarce resources (doctors time, medicines, hand gel etc.). 

If we prioritise the individual then the strongest rather than the neediest will win out.

In both cases, the pursuit of self-interest is inefficient, it undermines the overall response to the crisis and many more will die.

Research on emergencies (Drury et al., 2019) shows that it is precisely when people stop thinking in terms of ‘I’ and start thinking in terms of ‘we’ – more technically, when they develop a sense of shared social identity – that they start to coordinate, support each-other and ensure that the neediest get the greatest help.

Sometimes this sense of shared identity emerges by the very fact of experiencing a common threat. But messaging also matters. When a threat is framed in group rather than individual terms, the public response is more robust and more effective (Carter et al., 2013).

So .. Let’s Look Again At The COVID-19 Response.

Instead of personalising the issue we need to collectivise it.

The key issue is not so much ‘will I survive’ as ‘how do we get through it’.

The emphasis must lie on how we can act to ensure that the most vulnerable amongst us are protected and losses to the community are minimised – after all, from a collective perspective, a loss to one is a loss to all.

If framed in this way, then it becomes important for everyone to wash their hands and cover their coughs because of the implications for others as well as for themselves. 

Moreover, while we might have a right to take risks for ourselves, we have a moral obligation to avoid imposing risks on others (especially those who are vulnerable and connected to us – just think how your driving changes when you have children in the car).

Both of these considerations are powerful motivators of action (Reicher & Haslam, 2009). What is more, once certain actions become communal issues subject to collective norms, then violating them invokes collective pressure.

The best way to stop people going out when unwell or demanding resources they need less than others is not simply to change internal motivations but also to mobilise external disapproval.

The feverish person who goes to work, the fit young person demanding access to hospital emergency and admission services will be best dissuaded when the community comes together to make clear that these are not acceptable behaviours. 

Once you collectivise the response to COVID-19, and once you create clear norms about maximising community well-being, then you become less reliant upon external forces such as the police to regulate behaviours – say around who is prioritised in getting medical help – with all the risk of clashes that entails.Instead, the community itself will constrain would-be deviants in their midst.

As always, the best regulation is collective self-regulation (Reicher et al., 2004). 

The difficulty with this approach, of course, is that it is so much at odds with contemporary psychological commonsense, which insists that behaviour is governed by individual self interest.

It is also at odds with social changes which relentlessly undermine communities and collectivities, seek to transform social groups into individual consumers and view every relationship as a market based interpersonal exchange.  

Perhaps COVID-19 Is A Powerful Wake-Up Call.

We have to change the way we frame the epidemic. We have to change we see the individual and society.We have to collectivise – or we die.

A modified article by Stephen Reicher, University of St. Andrews.

References

John Drury, University of SussexReferencesCarter, H., Drury, J., Rubin, G., Williams, R. and Amlôt, R. (2013), "The effect of communication during mass decontamination", Disaster Prevention and Management, Vol. 22 No. 2, pp. 132-147. 
https://doi.org/10.1108/09653561311325280

Drury, J., Carter, H., Cocking, C., Ntontis, E., Tekin Guven, S., & Amlôt, R. (2019). Facilitating collective psychosocial resilience in the public in emergencies: Twelve recommendations based on the social identity approach. Frontiers in Public Health, 7 (141) doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2019.00141

Drury, J., & Alfadhli, K. (2019). Social identity, emergencies and disasters.  In R. Williams, S. Bailey, B. Kamaldeep, S. A. Haslam, C. Haslam, V. Kemp, & D. Maughan (Eds). Social scaffolding: Applying the lessons of contemporary social science to health, public mental health and healthcare. London: Royal College of Psychiatrists.

Drury, J., Cocking, C., & Reicher, S. (2009). The nature of collective resilience: Survivor reactions to the 2005 London bombings. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 27(1), 66-95.

Drury, J., Cocking, C., & Reicher, S. (2009). Everyone for themselves? A comparative study of crowd solidarity among emergency survivors. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48(3), 487-506.

Reicher, S. D., & Haslam, S. A. (2009). Beyond help: a social psychology of social solidarity and social cohesion. In M. Snyder, & S. Sturmer (Eds.), The Psychology of Prosocical Behaviour Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

Reicher, S., Stott, C., Cronin, P. and Adang, O. (2004), "An integrated approach to crowd psychology and public order policing", Policing: An International Journal, Vol. 27 No. 4, pp. 558-572. https://doi.org/10.1108/13639510410566271

Stott, C., Adang, O., Livingstone, A., & Schreiber, M. (2008). Tackling football hooliganism: A quantitative study of public order, policing and crowd psychology. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 14(2), 115-141. doi:10.1037/a0013419

Vilas, X., & Sabucedo, J. M. (2012). Moral obligation: A forgotten dimension in the analysis of collective action. Revista de Psicología Social, 27(3), 369-375.
Office Hours
Wednesday: 9am - 9pm
Thursday: 8am - 6pm
Friday: 8am - 6pm

CONTACT ME TODAY
or
SCHEDULE AN APPOINTMENT
Share by: